Running head: CENTRAL PLACE FORAGING THEORY AND PREY DISTRIBUTION




CENTRAL-PLACE FORAGING THEORY AND
AN ABRUPT PREY DISTRIBUTION LIMIT

John Landahl

Keywords: mussels, Mytilus edulis.







 


ABSTRACT

Although central-place foraging theory was originally developed for the case in which an individual predator forages out in all directions from a central location of some sort, its conclusions can be generalized to accommodate predator populations foraging out of refuges, not necessarily in all directions, and to infer the effects of their foraging on prey distributions within habitats. Other investigations have dealt with aspects of central-place foraging theory as it relates to predator behavior, but the present study indicates that its implications for prey distribution are also of interest.


INTRODUCTION

Well-defined (though often irregular) limits of the distributions of plants and of sessile animals within habitats (as opposed to larger geographic areas) are periodically observed in nature, sometimes in the absence of any obvious corresponding environmental discontinuity (e.g.,the lower margin of the mussel bed at Quartermaster Harbor described in Landahl 1985, Chapter III). In several cases experimental studies have demonstrated that distinct distributional boundaries occur at the border between partially overlapping predator and prey distributions. A well-known example is the lower limit of M. californianus in the rocky intertidal zone on the outer coast of Washington; this limit is set by sea star predation (Paine, 1974, 1976). From a theoretical standpoint, spatial distributions of predators and prey in a habitat can be disjoint or can overlap in several different ways (Figure 1) . When the predator and prey distributions are neither disjunct nor completely overlapping, the potential exists for the foraging activities of the predators to reduce prey density within a subset of the prey distribution.

Figure 1. Classes of within-habitat predator and prey distributions: disjoint (a), congruent (b), concentric with prey outside (c left), concentric with prey inside (c right), and overlapping (d).

In terrestrial systems, zones bare of vegetation have long been noted around shrubs in the California chaparral and coastal sage communities (e.g.,Whittaker, 1975; Ricklefs, 1980). These zones were originally interpreted as areas in which volatile toxins (e.g.,terpenes) produced by these shrubs (which include Salvia leucophylla, Adenostoma fasciculatum, Artemisia californica, and Baccharis pilularis) inhibited the growth of germinating seedlings (e.g.,Muller, Muller, and Haines, 1964; Muller and del Moral, 1966). However, Bartholomew (1970) used herbivore exclosures to show that these bare zones are maintained by the feeding activity of rodents, rabbits, and birds which take shelter in the shrubs but emerge (particularly at night) to browse or eat seeds in the surrounding grasslands.

Similar bare zones 1-2 m in width around clumps of the Argentinean thornbushes Schinus fasciculatus, Condalia microphylla, and Lycium gilliesianum are maintained by the foraging and trampling activities of the desert cavy (Microcavia australis), a small, ground-dwelling hystricomorph rodent (Rood, 1970).

Suchanek (1978, 1979) reported that distinct "halos" bare of the seaweed Fucus surround isolated mussel beds on rock hillocks in this habitat. Using field experiments, he showed that these zones, which are usually about 20 cm wide, were produced by the grazing activities of limpets which, at low tide, take shelter from desiccation within the mussel beds.

In the course of other studies (Landahl 1985, Chapter III), it became apparent that the intertidal mussel population at Quartermaster Harbor had an abrupt lower distributional boundary which did not coincide with any pronounced discontinuity in substrate (Landahl 1985). Experiments showed that this boundary was the result of predation by the kelp crab Pugettia producta (Landahl 1985, Chapter III). However, the reason for the abruptness of a boundary such as this was less clear. Woodin (1976) has suggested that intense adult-larval interactions can produce sharp boundaries between dense infaunal assemblages dominated by organisms of different functional types (e.g.,suspension-feeders, deposit-feeders). It seems likely that this mechanism could also produce sharp boundaries between dense beds of epifauna as well.

Hence one might hypothesize that the sharp lower margin of the mussel bed reflects the juncture between a dense epifaunal assemblage and a dense infaunal one; this will be referred to as the adult-larval interaction hypothesis. However, populations of infauna (or at least of larger infaunal bivalves) were not dense below the lower margin of the mussel beds (Landahl 1985, Chapter III), so the adult-larval interaction hypothesis does not appear satisfactory to explain the sharpness of the mussel distribution boundary.

An alternative hypothesis (here termed the "physiological tolerance" hypothesis) is that the predator's upper foraging limit is set by its physiological tolerances, and this hypothesis seems reasonable in the case of slow-moving predators susceptible to rapid desiccation (e.g.,sea stars). It is not surprising that the foraging activities of slow-moving predators poorly equipped to withstand desiccation (e.g.,sea stars, sea urchins) would set a distinct lower boundary for an intertidal mussel population as a result of their own physiological tolerances, but, for a variety of reasons, it is surprising that crab predation would set a similarly abrupt margin.

Unlike Evasterias troschelii and other sea stars known to prey on bivalves (Mauzey, Birkeland, and Dayton, 1968), P. producta moves rapidly. It is also highly resistant (though not completely invulnerable) to short-term desiccation by virtue of its chitinous exoskeleton. For this reason the upper limit of its foraging activities is not likely set by physiological tolerances. The width of the mussel bed at my study area was approximately 120 m (Landahl 1985). It is difficult to imagine that a sea star could traverse such a distance and return to the lower intertidal zone within one tidal cycle, but it is quite possible that a crab could migrate daily in this way if it were advantageous to do so. Carcinus maenas, a predatory crab reaching 7 cm in carapace width (Dare and Edwards, 1981), is capable of travelling up to 300 m upshore during high tides (Dare and Edwards, 1981). The portunid crab Scylla serrata found in S. Africa and Australia can move as much as 800 m during one night (Hill, 1978). Adult C. magister in Similk Bay, Wash., are estimated to move 92 m/day on the average (Mayer, 1973). Mass movements of crabs upshore on rising tides have been observed using underwater television (Dare and Edwards, 1981); corresponding movements downshore when the falling tide threatens to expose these animal also occur (Dare and Edwards, 1981).

Scoter ducks (which overwintered at Quartermaster Harbor; Chapter V) are known to prey on crabs (Cottam, 1939), and 10 to 20 Glaucous-winged Gulls and two Great Blue Herons were also present at this site for much of the year (Chapter V). In studies conducted in the Strait of Juan de Fuca and other inland marine waters of the State of Washington, Hirsch (1980) found that the diet of the Common Goldeneye (Bucephala clangula) included crabs of the genus Cancer and that the diet of the Oldsquaw (Clangula hyemalis) included P. producta. She found crustaceans of several species and unidentified crab claws in stomachs of White-winged and Surf Scoters, suggesting that, like Common Goldeneyes and Oldsquaws, the scoters may prey on these crabs as well.

Crabs may also be eaten by fish (e.g.,rockfish, sculpins) (Hines, 1982). However, rockfish and sculpins are not usually found in sandy or muddy areas, and only flatfish and salmon were seen during daytime snorkeling at Quartermaster Harbor.

Thus bird predation could have been expected to force crabs to retreat into the lower intertidal or the subtidal zones during daylight hours, and in fact I seldom observed crabs in the mussel bed during daylight snorkeling. In this case, the predators presumably only move far enough downward to escape their own predators, which are inferred to be diving ducks and Great Blue Herons. P. producta reaches densities of 1-2/m2 in a band comprising the rocky intertidal and shallow seagrass zones along the shore on the coast of California, but is almost absent in deeper water (Hines, 1982). At Quartermaster Harbor, P. producta could sometimes be found completely or partially burrowed into the sand in shallow water during daylight low tides, a behavior which suggested avoidance of visual predators. However, there was no obvious reason why crabs could not have invaded the interior of the mussel bed at night if they chose, and indeed, occasional instances of crab predation on mussels were observed well upshore of the lower margin.

A second alternative hypothesis (here termed the central-place foraging hypothesis) which would explain the abruptness of prey distribution boundaries resulting from predation is based on central-place foraging theory (Orians and Pearson, 1979; Orians, 1980). A central place, such as a nest, den, or colony site, is a location to which a predator returns after each foraging trip to store food, or feed young (Andersson, 1981). Central-place foraging theory is concerned with the energetic cost of travelling between this location and the foraging area and the amount of energy gained during the foraging period (Orians and Pearson, 1979; Orians, 1980).

Although central-place foraging theory was originally developed for the case in which an individual predator forages out in all directions from a central location of some sort, and has been applied primarily to aspects of foraging behavior such as patch choice and cheek-pouch loading in the Eastern chipmunk, Tamias striatus (Kramer and Nowell, 1980), search area in the Whinchat, Saxicola rubetra (Andersson, 1981), and hunting method choice and patch use in the American Kestrel, Falco sparverius (Rudolph, 1982), its conclusions can be generalized to accommodate predator populations foraging out of refuges, not necessarily in all directions, and to infer the effects of their foraging on prey distributions within habitats.

The central-place foraging hypothesis relating to prey distribution states that when a predator (or, presumably, a predator population) has a refuge or other central place (e.g a nest) within an otherwise uniform habitat which is occupied by a prey species, the foraging activities of that population will create a distinct and expanding zone largely or entirely free of optimal-size prey within a uniform distance from the refuge (Figure 2a). Woodin (1978) has discussed the role of refuges in ecological systems in detail.

Figure 2. Maintenance and expansion of a sharp-boundaried bare zone of uniform width by the foraging activities of a predator population utilizing a refuge. Arrows show the predators' outward movements during foraging; t1 is the width of the zone at time 1 and t2 is the width at time 2. When the refuge is a small central place, the bare zone is circular (a). When the refuge is the shallow subtidal zone along a shoreline, the shape of the bare zone is as shown in top view (b) and in cross-section (c).

The mechanism hypothesized to produce the uniformity of this bare zone ("halo" or "browse zone", in the terminology of Suchanek, 1978, 1979), with its sharp boundary, is foraging by the predator in a manner which minimizes its round-trip travel time and travel costs.

A case of this hypothesized phenomenon of particular interest to marine ecologists occurs when a predator on a sessile intertidal prey species takes periodic refuge from desiccation, from predation, or from both in the lower intertidal zone or in the shallow subtidal zone, resulting in a well-defined prey distribution boundary (Figure 2b,c). It has not been shown, however, that the upper limit of foraging by a predator taking periodic refuge in the lower intertidal zone is a result of minimizing either the time or cost of round-trip travel.

However, the central-place foraging hypothesis as applied to the intertidal zone (stating that lower intertidal predators forage upshore in such a way as to minimize round-trip travel time, or perhaps to minimize total feeding time; Schoener, 1971), appeared more satisfactory than the adult-larval interaction hypothesis or the physiological tolerance hypothesis to explain the abrupt M. edulis distribution boundary near the bottom of the middle intertidal zone at Quartermaster Harbor.


Discussion

The foraging behavior of P. producta has been little studied. However, Elner and Hughes (1978) studied foraging by C. maenas on isolated specimens of M. edulis in England in some detail. P. producta is a majid crab, while C. maenas is a cancrid. However, since the body sizes of the two species are similar, it is likely that the results obtained by Elner and Hughes (1978) can be extrapolated to P. producta. They concluded that C. maenas is a tactile predator, making use of neither chemosensory nor visual cues in locating its prey. S. serrata, on the other hand, is reported to detect infaunal and epifaunal bivalves by contact chemoreception, the dactyls of the walking legs being the location of the sensory receptors involved (Hill, 1979). Lack of reliance on visual cues is, of course, to be expected in nocturnal predators.

Elner and Hughes (1978) also found that for a crab of a particular size there is a mussel which is the optimal-size prey. Feeding on mussels of this size minimizes handling time and maximizes energy gain, smaller mussels having less energy content and larger ones taking disproportionately longer to handle and consume. It is likely that a P. producta of a given size also has an optimal prey size even when feeding on mussels in clumps rather than on isolated individuals.

However, because crabs have a wide repertoire of attack modes (Elner and Hughes, 1978; Williams, 1978; Seed, 1982), there is no maximum-size mussel within the length range 1-6.5 cm which an individual C. maenas cannot consume (Elner and Hughes, 1978). By resorting to edge-breaking (a slow attack mode) rather than crushing (a fast attack mode), even very large prey can be consumed. Since mussels at Quartermaster Harbor did not exceed 70 cm in shell length (Landahl 1985) and P. producta proved capable of consuming mussels at least 57 mm long in the laboratory, it is likely that the mussels at this locality had no size refuge from crab predation.

C. maenas adjusts rapidly to the prey size distribution it encounters in its foraging activities (Elner and Hughes, 1978). Thus crab predation would not necessarily be size-selective over a long time-span, since less-preferred prey would be consumed once the optimal-size mussels had been eaten. S. serrata also consumes a wide size-range of prey (Hill, 1979).

Most mussels at Quartermaster Harbor occurred in clumps, though a few isolated individuals were also present. Observations of feeding by a P. producta in a lab tank indicated that one of the first steps taken by this predator in attacking a mussel in a clump is use its chelae to snip the byssal threads attaching it to other mussels. This was sometimes a time-consuming process and could have resulted in a significant increase in handling time over that required for the isolated individual mussels used as prey items by Elner and Hughes (1978). Portunus puber also cuts the byssal threads of M. edulis to separate individuals from clumps, and Scylla serrata, a large Indo-West-Pacific portunid crab, likewise begins its attack on hairy mussels (Trichomya hirsuta) attached to rocks by breaking the byssal threads (Williams, 1978).

However, P. producta has smaller and less robust chelae than C. maenas. Elner (1980) investigated the effect of master chela size on the prey selection by C. maenas. C. maenas is sexually dimorphic in master chela size, with females possessing smaller master chelae than males of the same carapace width (Elner, 1980). However, some males also possess small master chelae, apparently as a result of accidental loss of the master chela as juveniles (Elner, 1980). Possession of a small master chela reduces the preferred size prey for a C. maenas of a given body size, suggesting that the optimal prey size for a P. producta may be smaller than for a C. maenas of the same carapace width or tissue dry weight. However, the conclusions of Elner and Hughes (1978) should still be applicable to P. producta in a qualitative way.

The effect of C. maenas predation on the size structure of mussel populations has not been established (Elner, 1980), although field experiments have shown that C. maenas predation influences mussel distribution in Great Britain (Kitching, Sloane, and Ebling, 1959; Ebling et al., 1964; Elner, 1980). C. maenas predation on the dogwhelk (Nucella lapillus) and on two littorinid snails (Littorina rudis and L. nigrolineata) is not size-selective due to the sparse distributions and unpredictable vulnerability of these prey (Hughes and Elner, 1979; Elner and Raffaelli, 1980).

At Quartermaster Harbor, the largest mussels in the bed were found at or near the lower margin (Landahl 1985), a common pattern in intertidal molluscs (Vermeij, 1972). The lower margin was also the optimal patch type for crab foraging because the mussels population was densest there (Landahl 1985) and because the mussels occurred in clumps which usually contained a full range of sizes from juveniles to adults of maximum size, presumably because some recruitment occurred at almost all times of year (Landahl 1985). Thus, no matter what the optimal-size mussel was for a given crab, that size would be encountered at the lower margin of the mussel bed. Moving higher would completely eliminate the possibility of encountering the largest prey, and would not greatly increase the probability of encountering prey of small or intermediate sizes. Small crabs could have foraged equally profitably at higher levels. However, since crabs retreated below the lower margin of the mussel bed during daylight hours and during low tides, foraging at the lower margin of the bed would have minimized travel time and travel costs for crabs of all sizes. It was also probable that it was advantageous for crabs to move the minimum possible distance upshore to feed in order to minimize the risk of predation on them by birds.

The hypothesis that P. producta foraged shoreward from its refuge in such a way as to minimize its round-trip travel time and costs or its total feeding time appeared to provide a more satisfactory explanation of the observations. It is difficult to distinguish between energy maximizing and time minimizing behavior in studies of crab foraging (Hughes and Seed, 1981). However, in future experiments it might be possible to differentiate between these two strategies by tethering crabs in the intertidal zone to determine whether they are subject to predation, e.g.,by birds such as scoters or great blue herons, and hence would benefit from minimizing the time spent feeding in that zone. When predators are themselves subject to predation by animals at higher trophic levels, the element of risk must be considered and time-minimizing behavior is expected to be favored over energy-maximizing behavior.

P. producta's preference for mussels near the lower margin of the bed at Quartermaster Harbor was parsimoniously explained on the basis of central-place foraging theory. Thus central-place foraging theory has utility in the study of marine ecological systems. In this case, it offered an explanation for two major static features of the mussel bed (the existence of the lower bed margin and its distinctness) and one dynamic aspect (the retreat of the boundary over time). It may be applicable to other situations as well. For example, the sea urchin Centrostephanus coronatus is another mobile marine invertebrate which forages at night to avoid predation (by the sheephead wrasse, Pimelometopon pulchrum, in this case) (Nelson and Vance, 1979), and also minimizes its travel time by staying near its refuge (holes and crevices in rock).

These results are of theoretical interest because they suggest that even when habitats lack structural refuges for prey (e.g.,holes, crevices, caves) and physical barriers which might impede the movement of predators, optimal foraging (e.g.,time-minimization or energy maximization) may result in the focussing of predation on a restricted portion of the prey habitat, producing phenomena such as the abrupt prey distribution limit at Quartermaster Harbor. General predator-prey models in the Lotka-Volterra lineage (e.g.,MacArthur, 1972; May, 1976) do not predict such abrupt prey distribution boundaries in the absence of physical barriers to prey or predator movement.

Other investigations (e.g.,Orians and Pearson, 1979; Orians, 1980; Kramer and Nowell, 1980 Andersson, 1981; Rudolph, 1982) have dealt with aspects of central-place foraging theory as it relates to predator behavior, but the present study indicates that its implications for prey distribution are also of interest.


Literature cited

Andersson, M. 1981. Central place foraging in the whinchat, Saxicola rubetra. Ecology 62: 538-544.


Bartholomew, B. 1970. Bare zone between California shrub and grassland communities: the role of animals. Science 170: 1210-1212.


Boulding, E. G. 1984. Crab-resistant features of shells of burrowing bivalves: decreasing vulnerability by increasing handling time. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 76. In press.


-----, and T. K. Hay. 1984. Crab response to prey density can result in density-dependent mortality of clams. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 41. In press.


Cottam, C. 1939. Food habits of North American diving ducks. U. S. Dept. Agri. Tech. Bull. 643. 139 p.


Dare, P. J., and D. B. Edwards. 1981. Underwater television observations on the intertidal movements of shore crabs, Carcinus maenas, across a mudflat. J. Mar. Biol. Assn. U. K. 61:107-116.


Ebling, F. J., J. A. Kitching, L. Muntz, and C. M. Taylor. 1964. The ecology of Lough Ine. XIII. Experimental observations on the destruction of Mytilus edulis and Nucella lapillus by crabs. J. Anim. Ecol. 33: 73-82.


Elner, R. W. 1980. The influence of temperature, sex, and chela size in the foraging strategy of the shore crab, Carcinus maenas (L.). Mar. Biol. Physiol. 7: 15-24.


-----, and R. N. Hughes. 1978. Energy maximization in the diet of the shore crab Carcinus maenas. J. Anim. Ecol. 47: 103-116.


-----, and D. G. Raffaelli. 1980. Interactions between two marine snails, Littorina rudis Maton and Littorina nigrolineata Gray, a predator, Carcinus maenas (L.), and a parasite Microphallus similis Jägerskiold. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 43: 151-160.


Hill, B. J. 1978. Activity, track and speed of movement of the crab Scylla serrata in an estuary. Mar. Biol. 47: 135-141.


-----. 1979. Aspects of the feeding strategy of the predatory crab Scylla serrata. Mar. Biol. 55: 209-214.


Hines, A. H. 1982. Coexistence in a kelp forest: size, population dynamics, and resource partitioning in a guild of spider crabs (Brachyura, Majidae). Ecol. Monogr. 52: 179-198.


Hirsch, K. V. 1980. Winter ecology of sea ducks in the inland marine waters of Washington. Seattle, WA: Univ. of Wash. 92 p. Thesis.


Hughes, R. N., and R. W. Elner. 1979. Tactics of a predator, Carcinus maenas, and morphological responses of the prey, Nucella lapillus. J. Anim. Ecol. 48: 65-78.


-----, and R. Seed. 1981. Size selection of mussels by the blue crab Callinectes sapidus. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 6: 83-89.


Kitching, J. A., J. F. Sloane, and F. J. Ebling. 1959. The ecology of Lough Ine. VIII. Mussels and their predators. J. Anim. Ecol. 28: 331-341.


Kramer, D. L., and W. Nowell. 1980. Central place foraging in the eastern chipmunk, Tamais striatus. Anim. Behav. 28: 772-778.


Landahl, J. T. 1985. Patterns of distribution of Mytilus edulis, their causes, and their effects on co-occurring fauna of a sand-gravel beach. Seattle, WA: Univ. of Wash. Dissertation. 337 p.


Landahl, J. T. 1988. Sediment-level fluctuation in a mussel bed on a protected sand-gravel beach. Estuarine, Coastal, and Shelf Science 26: 255-267. Keywords: sediment movement, mussels, Mytilus, storms.


MacArthur, R. H. 1972. Geographical ecology: patterns in the distribution of species. New York: Harper and Row. 269 p.


Mauzey, K. P., C. Birkeland, and P. K. Dayton. 1968. Feeding behavior of asteroids and escape responses of their prey in the Puget Sound region. Ecology 49: 603-619.


May, R. M. (ed.) 1976. Theoretical ecology: principles and applications. Philadephia: W. B. Saunders Co. 317 p.


Mayer, D. L. 1973. The ecology and thermal sensitivity of the Dungeness crab, Cancer magister, and related species of its benthic community in Similk Bay, Washington. Seattle, WA: Univ. of Wash. Dissertation. 188 p.


Muller, C. H., W. H. Muller, and B. L. Haines. 1964. Volatile growth inhibitors produced by aromatic shrubs. Science 143: 471-473.


-----, and R. del Moral. 1966. Soil toxicity induced by terpenes from Salvia leucophylla. Bull. Torrey Bot. Club 93: 130-137.


Nelson, B. V., and R. R. Vance. 1979. Diel foraging patterns of the sea urchin Centrostephanus coronatus as a predator avoidance strategy. Mar. Biol. 51: 251-258.


Newcombe, C. L. 1935. A study of the community relationships of the sea mussel, Mytilus edulis L. Ecology 16: 235-243.


Orians, G. H. 1980. Some adaptations of marsh-nesting blackbirds. Monographs in population biology No. 14. Princeton, N. J.: Princeton Univ. Press. 295 p.


-----, and N. E. Pearson. 1979. On the theory of central place foraging. Pages 155-177 in D. H. Horn, R. Mitchell, and G. R. Stairs (eds.). Analysis of ecological systems. Columbus, Ohio: Ohio State Univ. Press.


Paine, R. T. 1974. Intertidal community structure: experimental studies on the relationship between a dominant competitor and its principal predator. Oecologia 15: 93-120.


-----. 1976. Size-limited predation: an observational and experimental approach with the Mytilus-Pisaster interaction. Ecology 57: 858-873.


Ricklefs, R. E. 1979. Ecology (second ed.). New York: Chiron. 966 p.


Rood, J. P. 1970. Ecology and social behavior of the desert cavy (Microcavia australis). Am. Midland Nat. 83: 415-454.


Rudolph, S. G. 1982. Foraging strategies of American Kestrels during breeding. Ecology 63: 1268-1276.


Schoener, T. W. 1971. Theory of feeding strategies. Ann. Rev. Ecol. And Syst. 2: 369-404.


Seed, R. 1982. Predation of the ribbed mussel Geukensia demissa by the blue crab Callinectes sapidus. Neth. J. Sea Res. 16: 163-172.


Suchanek, T. H. 1978. The ecology of Mytilus edulis L. in exposed rocky intertidal communities. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 31: 105-120.


-----. 1979. The Mytilus californianus community: studies on the composition, structure, organization, and dynamics of a mussel bed. Seattle, WA: Univ. of Wash. Dissertation.


Vermeij, G. J. 1972. Intraspecific shore-level size gradients in intertidal molluscs. Ecology 53: 693-700.


Whittaker, R. H. 1975. Communities and ecosytems (2nd ed.). New York: Macmillan. 385 p.


Williams, M. J. 1978. Opening of bivalve shells by the mud crab Scylla serrata Forskal. Aust. J. Mar. Freshwater Res. 29: 699-702.


Woodin, S. A. 1976. Adult-larval interactions in dense infaunal assemblages: patterns of abundance. J. Mar. Res. 34: 25-41.


-----. 1978. Refuges, disturbance, and community structure: a marine soft-bottom example. Ecology 59: 274-284.


Original draft completed 30-Apr-84

Last substantive revision 10/7/90

HTML version last updated 11/4/97